<label id="dxwxx"><meter id="dxwxx"><bdo id="dxwxx"></bdo></meter></label>

<rt id="dxwxx"><small id="dxwxx"><strike id="dxwxx"></strike></small></rt><label id="dxwxx"><meter id="dxwxx"></meter></label>
<label id="dxwxx"><meter id="dxwxx"><bdo id="dxwxx"></bdo></meter></label>
  • <label id="dxwxx"><meter id="dxwxx"></meter></label>

  • <label id="dxwxx"><del id="dxwxx"></del></label>

    歡迎來到上海新航道學(xué)校官網(wǎng)!英語高能高分,就上新航道

    上海學(xué)校

    • 課程
    • 資訊

    4008-125-888

    雅思閱讀真題題庫之Neuroaesthetics原文+題目

    2017/6/2 17:54:57來源:新航道作者:新航道

    摘要:上海新航道培訓(xùn)學(xué)校小編給考生們帶來了雅思閱讀真題題庫之Neuroaesthetics原文+題目,希望備考雅思考試的同學(xué)們一定要認(rèn)真的看題、做題,多研究積累才能實(shí)現(xiàn)自我提升,預(yù)祝各位考生都取得理想的成績。

      上海新航道培訓(xùn)學(xué)校小編給考生們帶來了雅思閱讀真題題庫之Neuroaesthetics原文+題目,希望備考雅思考試的同學(xué)們一定要認(rèn)真的看題、做題,多研究積累才能實(shí)現(xiàn)自我提升,預(yù)祝各位考生都取得理想的成績。


      Neuroaesthetics

      An emerging discipline called neuroaesthetics is seeking to bring scientific objectivity to the study of art, and has already given us a better understanding of many masterpieces. The blurred imagery of Impressionist paintings seems to stimulate the brain’s amygdala, for instance. Since the amygdala plays a crucial role in our feelings, that finding might explain why many people find these pieces so moving.


      Could the same approach also shed light on abstract twentieth-century pieces, from Mondrian’s geometrical blocks of colour, to Pollock’s seemingly haphazard arrangements of splashed paint on canvas? Sceptics believe that people claim to like such works simply because they are famous. We certainly do have an inclination to follow the crowd. When asked to make simple perceptual decisions such as matching a shape to its rotated image, for example, people often choose a definitively wrong answer if they see others doing the same. It is easy to imagine that this mentality would have even more impact on a fuzzy concept like art appreciation, where there is no right or wrong answer.


      Angelina Hawley-Dolan, of Boston College, Massachusetts, responded to this debate by asking volunteers to view pairs of paintings — either the creations of famous abstract artists or the doodles of infants, chimps and elephants. They then had to judge which they preferred. A third of the paintings were given no captions, while many were labelled incorrectly — volunteers might think they were viewing a chimp’s messy brushstrokes when they were actually seeing an acclaimed masterpiece. In each set of trials, volunteers generally preferred the work of renowned artists, even when they believed it was by an animal or a child. It seems that the viewer can sense the artist’s vision in paintings, even if they can’t explain why.


      Robert Pepperell, an artist based at Cardiff University, creates ambiguous works that are neither entirely abstract nor clearly representational. In one study, Pepperell and his collaborators asked volunteers to decide how ‘powerful’ they considered an artwork to be, and whether they saw anything familiar in the piece. The longer they took to answer these questions, the more highly they rated the piece under scrutiny, and the greater their neural activity. It would seem that the brain sees these images as puzzles, and the harder it is to decipher the meaning, the more rewarding is the moment of recognition.


      And what about artists such as Mondrian, whose paintings consist exclusively of horizontal and vertical lines encasing blocks of colour? Mondrian’s works are deceptively simple, but eye-tracking studies confirm that they are meticulously composed, and that simply rotating a piece radically changes the way we view it. With the originals, volunteers’ eyes tended to stay longer on certain places in the image, but with the altered versions they would flit across a piece more rapidly. As a result, the volunteers considered the altered versions less pleasurable when they later rated the work.


      In a similar study, Oshin Vartanian of Toronto University asked volunteers to compare original paintings with ones which he had altered by moving objects around within the frame. He found that almost everyone preferred the original, whether it was a Van Gogh still life or an abstract by Miro. Vartanian also found that changing the composition of the paintings reduced activation in those brain areas linked with meaning and interpretation.


      In another experiment, Alex Forsythe of the University of Liverpool analysed the visual intricacy of different pieces of art, and her results suggest that many artists use a key level of detail to please the brain. Too little and the work is boring, but too much results in a kind of ‘perceptual overload’; according to Forsythe. What’s more, appealing pieces both abstract and representational, show signs of ‘fractals’ — repeated motifs recurring in different scales. Fractals are common throughout nature, for example in the shapes of mountain peaks or the branches of trees. It is possible that our visual system, which evolved in the great outdoors, finds it easier to process such patterns.


      It is also intriguing that the brain appears to process movement when we see a handwritten letter, as if we are replaying the writer’s moment of creation. This has led some to wonder whether Pollock’s works feel so dynamic because the brain reconstructs the energetic actions the artist used as he painted. This may be down to our brain’s ‘mirror neurons’, which are known to mimic others’ actions. The hypothesis will need to be thoroughly tested, however. It might even be the case that we could use neuroaesthetic studies to understand the longevity of some pieces of artwork. While the fashions of the time might shape what is currently popular, works that are best adapted to our visual system may be the most likely to linger once the trends of previous generations have been forgotten.


      It’s still early days for the field of neuroaesthetics — and these studies are probably only a taste of what is to come. It would, however, be foolish to reduce art appreciation to a set of scientific laws. We shouldn’t underestimate the importance of the style of a particular artist, their place in history and the artistic environment of their time. Abstract art offers both a challenge and the freedom to play with different interpretations. In some ways, it’s not so different to science, where we are constantly looking for systems and decoding meaning so that we can view and appreciate the world in a new way.


    雅思在線免費(fèi)模考


      Questions 27-30

      Choose the correct letter, A, B, C or D.

      Write the correct letter in boxes 27-30 on your answer sheet.


      27 In the second paragraph, the writer refers to a shape-matching test in order to illustrate

      A the subjective nature of art appreciation.

      B the reliance of modern art on abstract forms.

      C our tendency to be influenced by the opinions of others.

      D a common problem encountered when processing visual data.


      28 Angelina Hawley-Dolan’s findings indicate that people

      A mostly favour works of art which they know well.

      B hold fixed ideas about what makes a good work of art.

      C are often misled by their initial expectations of a work of art.

      D have the ability to perceive the intention behind works of art.


      29 Results of studies involving Robert Pepperell’s pieces suggest that people

      A can appreciate a painting without fully understanding it.

      B find it satisfying to work out what a painting represents.

      C vary widely in the time they spend looking at paintings.

      D generally prefer representational art to abstract art.


      30 What do the experiments described in the fifth paragraph suggest about the paintings of Mondrian?

      A They are more carefully put together than they appear.

      B They can be interpreted in a number of different ways.

      C They challenge our assumptions about shape and colour.

      D They are easier to appreciate than many other abstract works.


      Questions 31-33

      Complete the summary using the list of words, A-H, below.

      Write the correct letters, A-H, in boxes 31-33 on your answer sheet.

      Art and the Brain

      The discipline of neuroaesthetics aims to bring scientific objectivity to the study of art. Neurological studies of the brain, for example, demonstrate the impact which Impressionist paintings have on our 31 __________. Alex Forsythe of the University of Liverpool believes many artists give their works the precise degree of 32 __________ which most appeals to the viewer’s brain. She also observes that pleasing works of art often contain certain repeated 33 __________ which occur frequently in the natural world.

      A interpretation B complexity C emotions

      D movements E skill F layout

      G concern H images


      Questions 34-39

      Do the following statements agree with the views of the writer in Reading Passage 3?

      In boxes 34-39 on your answer sheet, write

      YES if the statement agrees with the views of the writer

      NO if the statement contradicts the views of the writer

      NOT GIVEN if there is no information on this

      34 Forsythe’s findings contradicted previous beliefs on the function of ‘fractals’ in art.

      35 Certain ideas regarding the link between ‘mirror neurons’ and art appreciation require further verification.

      36 People’s taste in paintings depends entirely on the current artistic trends of the period.

      37 Scientists should seek to define the precise rules which govern people’s reactions to works of art.

      38 Art appreciation should always involve taking into consideration the cultural context in which an artist worked.

      39 It is easier to find meaning in the field of science than in that of art.


      Question 40

      Choose the correct letter, A, B, C or D.

      Write the correct letter in box 40 on your answer sheet.

      40 What would be the most appropriate subtitle for the article?

      A Some scientific insights into how the brain responds to abstract art

      B Recent studies focusing on the neural activity of abstract artists

      C A comparison of the neurological bases of abstract and representational art

      D How brain research has altered public opinion about abstract art

      以上就是小編整理的雅思閱讀真題題庫原文,更多雅思閱讀資料,請點(diǎn)擊:雅思閱讀頻道

    免費(fèi)獲取資料

    熱報(bào)課程

    • 雅思課程
    班級名稱 班號 開課時(shí)間 人數(shù) 學(xué)費(fèi) 報(bào)名

    免責(zé)聲明
    1、如轉(zhuǎn)載本網(wǎng)原創(chuàng)文章,情表明出處
    2、本網(wǎng)轉(zhuǎn)載媒體稿件旨在傳播更多有益信息,并不代表同意該觀點(diǎn),本網(wǎng)不承擔(dān)稿件侵權(quán)行為的連帶責(zé)任;
    3、如本網(wǎng)轉(zhuǎn)載稿、資料分享涉及版權(quán)等問題,請作者見稿后速與新航道聯(lián)系(電話:021-64380066),我們會第一時(shí)間刪除。

    制作:每每

    旗艦校區(qū):上海徐匯區(qū)文定路209號寶地文定商務(wù)中心1樓 乘車路線:地鐵1/4號線上海體育館、3/9號線宜山路站、11號線上海游泳館站

    電話:4008-125-888

    版權(quán)所有:上海胡雅思投資管理有限公司 滬ICP備11042568號-1

    主站蜘蛛池模板: 亚洲AV日韩精品久久久久久久| 久久久久久久亚洲Av无码| 波多野结衣在线免费观看| 成年轻人网站色免费看| 免费va在线观看| 亚洲国产二区三区久久| 亚洲中文字幕无码亚洲成A人片| 午夜不卡AV免费| 无码少妇精品一区二区免费动态 | 中文亚洲成a人片在线观看| 亚洲一本综合久久| 日韩免费在线视频| 亚洲乱码中文字幕手机在线| 亚洲黄色网址在线观看| 精品成人一区二区三区免费视频| 最近2019免费中文字幕6| jjzz亚洲亚洲女人| 亚洲色中文字幕在线播放| 日韩免费视频在线观看| 亚洲欧洲国产精品你懂的| 亚洲毛片免费视频| 九月丁香婷婷亚洲综合色| 污视频网站在线免费看| 久久久久久久免费视频| 久久91亚洲精品中文字幕| h在线观看视频免费网站| 91麻豆国产自产在线观看亚洲 | 亚洲视频免费一区| 成年大片免费视频| 乱人伦中文视频在线观看免费| 免费视频成人片在线观看| 亚洲自偷自偷图片| 亚洲一区免费视频| 国产亚洲高清在线精品不卡| 久久精品国产亚洲沈樵| 国产美女在线精品免费观看| 亚洲免费视频播放| 7723日本高清完整版免费| 男人和女人高潮免费网站| 亚洲视频小说图片| 亚洲精品WWW久久久久久|